logo

48 pages 1 hour read

Nicholas Carr

The Shallows: What the Internet Is Doing to Our Brains

Nonfiction | Book | Adult | Published in 2010

A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.

Chapter 7-Digression 4Chapter Summaries & Analyses

Chapter 7 Summary: “The Juggler’s Brain”

Having explored the intellectual history of the Internet as a technology, Carr turns to “the crucial question: What can science tell us about the actual effects that Internet use is having on the way our minds work?” (115). The Internet engages a person’s sense of sight, hearing, and touch while providing immediate rewards for inputs: Clicking links opens new pages, liking a post creates a heart image, and clicking a thumbnail opens a video. This interactivity also provides social reinforcement, connecting people immediately through messaging, images, and videos. The Internet simultaneously engages the mind while also keeping it from focusing deeply on any one bit of content for longer than a few minutes. 

Distraction affects how information moves from working memory—the conscious mind—to long-term memory, the “seat of understanding” where a person’s knowledge is organized and connected in schemas (124). While long-term memory has theoretically unlimited storage capacity, the working memory, a person’s cognitive load, can be overwhelmed by information in the moment. Two activities that create cognitive overload are “‘extraneous problem-solving’ and ‘divided attention […] central features of the Net as an information medium” (125). Carr summarizes several educational psychology experiments demonstrating that people learn more effectively through linear printed media than through hypertext multimedia because of cognitive overload. When people read hypertext multimedia like websites, they scan the page quickly, rather than reading methodically, resulting in a shallow understanding of the content. Carr quotes library science professor Ziming Liu, whose research demonstrates that hyperlinks cause distraction and impede sustained thinking.

Not all the effects of Internet media are negative. The shift from linear, deep reading to power skimming develops people’s “hand eye coordination, reflex response, and the processing of visual cues” and potentially results in “a small expansion in the capacity of our working memory” (139). Referencing Patricia Greenfield’s 2009 article on “the effect of different types of media on people’s intelligence and learning ability” (141), Carr concludes that each type of media contributes to the strengthening of some cognitive abilities at the expense of others. In 2009, Stanford researchers tested multitaskers’ ability to control and direct their attention and found that heavy multitaskers, those engaged in the kind of activity encouraged by Internet use, struggled to discern important information from irrelevant information during research, “sacrificing performance on the primary task to let in other sources of information” (142). Mental functions that enable self-reflection and high-level attention control decrease as Internet use increases.

Digression 3 Summary: “A Digression: On the Buoyancy of IQ Scores”

The third digression discusses the Flynn effect—a steady rise in IQ scores throughout the 20th century, named for political scientist James Flynn who conducted the study that identified this trend. The IQ test specifically measures spatial reasoning and pattern recognition. Average scores on other measures of intelligence, such as those testing general knowledge, vocabulary, and arithmetic, have shown a small decline. Since he first described the Flynn effect, Flynn has continued researching general intelligence, and in a 2007 interview, he explained his conclusion: IQ scores saw a rise specifically because “abstract reasoning moved into the mainstream” with the advent of electronic media and Internet use (147).

Chapter 8 Summary: “The Church of Google”

The Internet is designed for “the efficient, automated collection, transmission, and manipulation of information” (150), and the company Google, founded in 1996, created a search engine for the World Wide Web that could catalog all the pages on the Internet. In 2000, Google began to derive the bulk of its revenues from advertising tied to relevant search results. To serve its company’s best interests, Google has consistently invested in data centers, freedom of information, and the efficiency and velocity of people’s interaction with the Internet. Google’s goal is to constantly increase the amount of information available on the Internet and the ease with which users can access that information; it has begun a project of digitizing all print media to make it searchable and manipulable through Google Book Search. Opponents of Google Book Search fear that the project breaks copyright law and could allow Google to restrict information access in the future. Paraphrasing a passage from the journal of 19th-century American novelist Nathaniel Hawthorne, Carr compares Google, with its focus on efficiency and speed, to the locomotive that breaks Hawthorne’s contemplative idyll as he picnics in Sleepy Hollow, New York. Carr notes that the historian Leo Marx made the same link in 1964 when discussing the advent of television—the idea that new technologies pose a threat to peace and contemplation is nothing new. American Transcendentalists like Hawthorne, Emerson, and Thoreau believed that “true enlightenment comes only through contemplation and introspection” (167). They experienced urbanization and industrialization as disruptive to those mental efforts. In Carr’s present, accelerating intellectual technologies create the same kind of disruption. He describes how Google’s ability to categorize large quantities of information hasn’t lessened the effect of information overload but exacerbated it. Google’s founder, Larry Page, and other computer scientists have suggested that the solution to the human brain’s problems with inattention, confusion, or processing could be solved by artificial intelligence.

Chapter 9 Summary: “Search, Memory”

Prior to the development of digital media, memorization was instrumental to reflection and learning. As computers made information immediately accessible, memorization seemed less necessary; Carr quotes a variety of writers and thinkers who have referred to computers as a second brain, or a “substitute for personal memory” (echoing Plato’s claim about writing itself in the Phaedrus), but he then references a number of scientific studies that contradict these claims (181). Scientist Louis Flexner discovered that long-term memory involves synthesis of proteins; Eric Kandel discovered that moving information from short-term to long-term memory increases the number of synapses in the brain; and the Columbia molecular cognition research group, led by Kandel, discovered how the chemical serotonin triggers the growth of new synaptic terminals, leading to stronger memory. Scientist Kobi Rosenblum contrasts human memory and computer memory: “While an artificial brain absorbs information and immediately saves it in its memory, the human brain continues to process information long after it is received, and the quality of memories depends on how the information is processed” (191). The Internet, rather than expanding working memory, creating space for long-term memory and learning, puts pressure on working memory, and “[obstructs] the consolidation of long-term memories and the development of schemas” (193). Conscious attention initiates and sustains the chemical process of long-term memory consolidation, so disruptions to attention—distractions—limit how well memory is processed. Carr references Williams James’s 1892 lecture on memory, in which James argued that the connections formed in the brain as working memory transitions into long-term memory are the foundation of human culture; “the connecting is thinking […] the connecting is the self” (195).

Digression 4 Summary: “A Digression: On the Writing of This Book”

Carr explains how writing The Shallows was difficult at first, until he disconnected from the distractions of social media, notifications, and browsing. He notes that his ability to disconnect for a time is a form of privilege in a world where professional and social lives are intertwined with the Internet. He confesses that despite the learning he has experienced through writing the book, he still chooses to engage with the Internet: “I have to confess: it’s cool. I’m not sure I could live without it” (200).

Chapter 7-Digression 4 Analysis

In this section of The Shallows, Carr explores The Nature of Memory and Learning in the Digital Era. Rather than address the neuroscience of Internet use earlier in the book, Carr reserves this discussion for after his history of intellectual technology because “the Net is best understood as the latest in a long series of tools that have helped mold the human mind” (115). Immediately, Carr connects the coming analysis of the Internet’s effects on memory and learning to his earlier explanation of how book technology affected human cognition. The Internet deeply engages a person’s sight, hearing, and touch, as well as a person’s sense of community; web surfers “display extensive activity across all brain regions […] the need to evaluate links and make related navigational choices, while also processing a multiplicity of fleeting sensory stimuli, requires constant mental coordination […] distracting the brain” (122). Carr relies on a bevy of academic references to support his claim that the inherently distracting nature of the Internet as a medium negatively impacts learning and memory.

The argument laid out across Chapters 7-9 relies on both logos—using logic and evidence to support claims—and ethos—a sense of trust in the author and his sources. In these chapters, Carr proposes that the Internet has a negative impact on cognition because it creates cognitive overload, “[impeding] deep learning and thinking” (126). Rather than use anecdotal evidence from his own life as he does earlier in the text, Carr supports each element of his claim with academic research. First, he references a 1990s experiment that proved electronic documents created more cognitive load, and then he explains a series of 2000s experiments that proved a higher cognitive load impedes comprehension and problem-solving. He uses academic research to address the suggestion that despite these effects on cognition, Internet use has developed other, more important skills, like multitasking. Carr’s rebuttal, like his primary argument, relies on appeals to scientific authority, quoting psychologist Patricia Greenfield and researchers at Stanford whose experiments suggest that muti-tasking is not effective. Digression 3’s discussion of IQ scores serves a similar function in Carr’s argument, disproving a commonly held belief in support of Internet use by referencing scientific evidence. 

Carr specifically addresses the neuroscience of learning in Chapter 9, rebutting the belief that human memory is analogous to computer memory. The previous chapter establishes Larry Page and Google as symbols of this metaphor: “What’s disturbing about the company’s founders is not their boyish desire to create an amazingly cool machine […] but the pinched conception of the human mind that gives rise to such a desire” (176). In connecting Page to the characterization of human learning and memory that Carr will go on to refute in Chapter 9, Carr dodges defensiveness in his reader. As he systematically disproves the metaphor of the human brain as a computer, he does so in conversation with Page, rather than with the reader directly. This allows the reader, who may very well also believe that the human brain is like a computer, to correct their thinking without feeling directly confronted by Carr. He deploys the same rhetorical strategy in Digression 4 when he admits that after finishing The Shallows, he’s “already backsliding […] [he’s] jacked into [his] RSS feed again […] [he] broke down and bought a Blu-ray player with built-in Wi-fi connection” (200). The conversational, confessional tone, coupled with the first-person perspective, frames Carr’s rebuttal against Page, scientific management, and the power of multi-tasking as a personal struggle for him, rather than a failing or flaw in the reader.

blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
blurred text